Empirical Research

The Link Between Math Anxiety and Math Achievement: The Role of Afterschool Learning

Seowon Song1, Tianyu Li2, Michaela Quintero3, Zhe Wang*4

Journal of Numerical Cognition, 2023, Vol. 9(3), 418–432, https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.11325

Received: 2023-02-07. Accepted: 2023-10-09. Published (VoR): 2023-11-30.

Handling Editor: Colleen M. Ganley, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA

*Corresponding author at: Department of Educational Psychology, Texas A&M University, Harrington Education Center Office Tower, 4225 TAMU, 540 Ross Street, College Station, TX 77843, USA. E-mail: zhe.wang@tamu.edu

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

The present study tested the learning avoidance model by examining the degree to which learning avoidance in various afterschool settings mediated the negative association between math anxiety and math achievement. Participants consisted of 207 third to sixth graders. Using a path model, findings showed that students’ math anxiety was negatively associated with both standardized math achievement test scores and parent-reported math school grades. Additionally, higher math anxiety was associated with more negative homework behaviors and less frequent participation in math-related extracurricular activities. Finally, the association between math anxiety and math achievement was partially mediated by negative math homework behaviors and participation in math extracurricular activities. Effort in math exam preparation did not contribute to explaining the association between math anxiety and math achievement. Overall, these findings support the learning avoidance model and suggest that avoidance behaviors in everyday learning in the afterschool setting may contribute to explaining the undesired math achievement among highly math anxious students.

Keywords: math anxiety, math homework behaviors, math extracurricular activities, learning avoidance model, elementary and middle school

Math anxiety is a well-documented risk factor for negative math learning outcomes in various educational stages (Barroso et al., 2021; Namkung et al., 2019). Several theories have been put forth to explain the observed associations between math anxiety and negative math learning outcomes (Carey et al., 2016; Ramirez et al., 2018), among which the learning avoidance model argues that math anxiety impairs math achievement by way of fostering learning avoidance in math related situations. Yet, relatively few studies empirically tested this model, in particular with regard to how daily learning behaviors may contribute to explaining the negative math anxiety – math achievement association in early educational stages. The present study aims to fill this major gap in the literature by investigating the mediating roles of a variety of learning behaviors in everyday afterschool settings, such as homework behaviors, exam preparations, and extracurricular activity participation, in the association between math anxiety and math achievement in elementary and middle school students.

Theoretical Models for the Math Anxiety and Math Achievement Association

In general, math anxiety is modestly negatively associated with math achievement (Barroso et al., 2021; Namkung et al., 2019). A few models have been put forth to explain the mechanism underlying this negative association (Carey et al., 2016; Ramirez et al., 2018). The deficit model argues that students with lower math abilities experience more difficulties in math learning which induces higher math anxiety (Carey et al., 2016; Ma & Xu, 2004; Tobias, 1986). The learning avoidance model argues that students with higher math anxiety are more likely to avoid math learning opportunities which contributes to their lower math skill and knowledge levels (Hembree, 1990; LeFevre et al., 1992). The cognitive interference model argues that worries or intrusive thoughts experienced by students with high math anxiety disrupt their working memory capacities temporarily during math problem solving, which undermines student’s math performance (Ramirez et al., 2013). Finally, the reciprocal model argues that math achievement and math anxiety mutually influence each other such that lower math achievement elicits higher subsequent math anxiety, which in turn contributes to even lower math achievement (Cargnelutti et al., 2017; Luo et al., 2014). The present study focuses on investigating the mechanism proposed by the learning avoidance model.

Empirical Evidence for the Learning Avoidance Model and Remaining Gaps

There are different ways in which students may avoid math learning. For example, students may avoid math learning completely in some situations, such as not taking math electives in high school or pursuing math-heavy majors in college (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001; Hembree, 1990; LeFevre et al., 1992). In situations where complete avoidance is not an option, they may exhibit avoidance in the form of negative learning behaviors, such as mentally disengaging, investing little effort in math learning, and spending little time on math work (Geary et al., 2021; Hasty et al., 2021; Quintero et al., 2022). Using this broad definition of learning avoidance, several studies have provided preliminary evidence supporting the learning avoidance model. A few studies found that students with higher math anxiety were less likely to take math related courses in high school and college or to explore math-related careers (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001; Hembree, 1990; LeFevre et al., 1992). While these studies focused on investigating distal avoidance behaviors in high school and college students, several recent studies examined how everyday learning behaviors contributed to the low math achievement among highly math anxious elementary to high school students (Geary et al., 2021; Hasty et al., 2021; Quintero et al., 2022). Specifically, Quintero and colleagues (2022) examined the role of classroom engagement in the negative association between math anxiety and math achievement in elementary and middle school students. They found that students with higher math anxiety were less engaged cognitively and behaviorally in math class, which in turn accounted for their lower math performance. Similarly, a study in a sample of middle school students found that students with higher math anxiety were less attentive in math class than students with lower math anxiety (Geary et al., 2021). Overall, these studies support the learning avoidance hypothesis by revealing that students with higher math anxiety exhibit avoidance in math class by being less attentive and engaged than their non-anxious peers.

The present study aims to contribute to this slowly growing literature by addressing two important gaps. First, most existing studies that investigated the learning avoidance model used samples of high school and college students. Individual differences in math anxiety emerge as early as kindergarten or first grade (Ganley & McGraw, 2016; Lu et al., 2021), which points to the potentially long-lasting negative implications of early development of math anxiety. Therefore, it is critical to elucidate the mechanism through which math anxiety may impair math learning in early educational stages. The present study aims to test the learning avoidance model in a sample of elementary and middle school students.

Second, whether highly math anxious students avoid math learning beyond the classroom setting is not well understood. Afterschool learning critically complements learning in the classroom – it is when students consolidate their knowledge through reviewing class materials, practicing learned skills, seeking for additional resources to address knowledge gaps, and applying classroom learning in various extracurricular activities (Cooper, 2007; Feldman & Matjasko, 2005). Importantly, the afterschool setting may represent a more informal and self-paced learning environment compared to the classroom setting. In the classroom, students often face situations such as taking tests under timed conditions and solving problems in front of other students, which can be particularly anxiety-provoking for some students (Chiu & Henry, 1990; Hopko et al., 2003; Lukowski et al., 2019). In contrast, students may have a better sense of control over the content, pace, duration, as well as the physical and social environment of learning in the afterschool setting. As such, examining the afterschool learning behaviors among highly math anxious students would inform us the degree of pervasiveness of their learning avoidance across a variety of learning contexts.

To our best knowledge, there have been only two studies that investigated students’ afterschool learning behaviors in relation to their math anxiety levels (Hasty et al., 2021; Jenifer et al., 2022). The Hasty et al. (2021) study investigated the quantity of middle and high school students’ afterschool math learning, which was operationalized by the amount of time students spent on doing math homework and taking out-of-school math lessons. They found that highly math anxious students spent a similar amount of time as their non-anxious peers studying math in the afterschool setting. However, given that only the amount of learning time was examined, it is unclear whether the quality of afterschool learning differs among students with different levels of math anxiety. The study conducted by Jenifer and colleagues (2022) examined exam preparation strategies in a sample of high school students and found that students with higher math anxiety were less likely to engage in effortful leaning strategies during exam preparation, and the difference in learning strategies contributed to performance differences among students with different levels of math anxiety (Jenifer et al., 2022).

The Present Study

The present study aims to extend the Hasty et al. (2021) and Jenifer et al. (2022) studies in three ways. First, we aim to examine learning behaviors in a variety of afterschool settings, including during homework, exam preparation, and participation in math extracurricular activities, regarding how they may contribute to the negative association between math anxiety and math achievement. These three contexts were chosen to cover a diverse range of afterschool learning activities that are associated with students’ achievement outcomes, which will be discussed in more details below. Second, instead of focusing on the amount of time students spend on homework as in the Hasty et al. (2021) study, we investigated the quality of homework time by examining behaviors such as procrastinating, rushing through homework, and failing to complete homework, as it is the quality of homework time that contributes to knowledge gains (Fan et al., 2017; Rosário et al., 2018; Trautwein, 2007; Xu et al., 2021). Finally, we extend the current literature by utilizing a sample of third to sixth graders, which allows us to better understand the applicability of the learning avoidance model in accounting for the early emerging association between math anxiety and math achievement.

Among the three afterschool learning settings, homework provides students with opportunities to review, practice, and apply the materials learned in class (Cooper, 2007), and has been found to contribute to students’ achievement development (Fan et al., 2017). When different dimensions of homework behaviors are examined, time spent on homework is often found to not correlate or correlate negatively with achievement scores, whereas homework effort correlates positively with achievement gains (Fan et al., 2017; Rosário et al., 2018; Trautwein, 2007; Xu et al., 2021). These findings suggest that the quality, not quantity, of homework behaviors contributes to positive learning outcomes. Similarly, exams are important evaluative experiences that often induce a protracted sequence of preparation involving review and knowledge integration (Eckerlein et al., 2019; Moneta & Spada, 2009). Accordingly, students who exhibit higher levels of perseverance and use of effortful and deep-processing strategies during exam preparation are found to have better exam performance (Ainley, 1993; Elliot et al., 1999; Jenifer et al., 2022; McGregor & Elliot, 2002). Finally, extracurricular activities are often defined as adult-supervised structured activities that take place outside of school hours (Luthar et al., 2006). They may include a variety of informal academic and nonacademic activities that offer students opportunities to develop intellectual, social, and physical skills (Feldman & Matjasko, 2005). Children often select extracurricular activities based on their skills, interests, values, and self-perceived abilities (Jacobs et al., 2005; Simpkins et al., 2010). In turn, the diversity and frequency of the extracurricular activities that students participate in contribute to their positive development in the academic and social domains during childhood and adolescence (Feldman & Matjasko, 2005; Metsäpelto & Pulkkinen, 2014). In particular, more frequent participation in academic extracurricular activities is believed to foster cognitive (e.g., critical thinking) and noncognitive (e.g., positive academic motivation) skills that promote academic successes (Covay & Carbonaro, 2010; Knifsend & Graham, 2012; Morris, 2016), which subsequently contribute to the development of a series of positive academic outcomes, including better academic performance, lower dropout rates, and higher academic aspirations (Broh, 2002; Fredricks, 2012; Haghighat & Knifsend, 2019; Mahoney & Cairns, 1997; Metsäpelto & Pulkkinen, 2014).

In summary, despite the well-documented negative association between math anxiety and math achievement in elementary and middle school students, few studies examined the degree to which avoidant learning behaviors may explain this negative association. Given the paucity of research testing the learning avoidance model in early educational stages, the present study addresses this research gap by investigating, in a sample of elementary and middle school students, (1) the degree to which students with high math anxiety avoid math learning in a variety of afterschool learning contexts including homework, exam preparation, and participation in math extracurricular activities, and (2) whether avoidance in these contexts mediate the negative association between math anxiety and math achievement. We measured math achievement using two different approaches to capture the possible differences between academic gains through the diverse afterschool learning activities. For example, students’ course grades largely capture what is learned in the classroom, whereas standardized achievement tests may additionally assess students’ academic skills beyond their curricula (Popham, 1999; Willingham et al., 2002). As such, effort spent on homework and exam preparation may be more strongly related to course grades than standardized test scores, whereas participation in extracurricular activities may improve standardized achievement test scores more than course grades (Duckworth et al., 2012).

Based on the learning avoidance model, we hypothesized that higher math anxiety would be associated with more avoidance behaviors in afterschool math learning (e.g., more negative homework behaviors such as procrastination or failure to complete homework, less perseverance in exam preparation, and less frequent participation in various math extracurricular activities). Additionally, we hypothesized that these avoidance behaviors in afterschool math learning would mediate the negative association between math anxiety and math achievement.

Method

Participants

Participants (N = 207 students; 50% female) were from an IRB-approved ongoing longitudinal study, which was designed to examine the affective processes in math learning. Families were recruited via digital advertisement, flyers, and community events from a city located in West Texas. The mean age of students was 10.18 years (SD = 1.04). Participating students were in Grades 3 to 6, but they were primarily in Grades 4-6 (2% third graders; 37% fourth graders; 37% fifth graders; 24% sixth graders). In terms of participants’ family background: parents of 10% of the participating students received high school degree or below, 18% attended college without graduating, 35% graduated from college, 5% attended graduate or professional school without graduating, and 31% received graduate or professional school degrees. In terms of family annual income, 9% families fell below $20,000, 14% were within the $20,000 – $40,000 range, 18% were within the $40,000 – $60,000 range, 14% were within the $60,000 – $80,000 range, 19% were within the $80,000 – $100,000 range, 15% were within the $100,000 – 150,000 range, 10% were within the $150,000 – $200,000 range, and 2% fell above $200,000. In terms of race, 68% were White, 8% were African American, 6% were Asian, 2% were Native American or Alaska Native, and 15% others. As of ethnicity, 38% were Hispanic. According to parental report, 16 (8%) students were diagnosed with one or more developmental disabilities, such as autism, dyslexia, ADHD, and ODD.

Procedure

Each family visited the lab for three hours for their initial assessment. Participants first completed the consent and assent forms. The child then completed a series of computerized math and neurocognitive tasks that are not used in the present study. Then, each child completed the standardized achievement testing. Finally, each child completed a series of questionnaires. The parent first completed the standardized achievement tests, which are not used in the present study. They subsequently filled out a series of questionnaires. Finally, they completed a few computerized math and neurocognitive tasks that are not used in the present study.

Measures

Math Anxiety

Students’ math anxiety was assessed with the 22 items of the Mathematics Anxiety Scale for Children (Chiu & Henry, 1990). Students provided their reports on how anxious they felt in various math-related situations on a 4-point scale (1 = not nervous to 4 = very, very nervous). An example item is “Thinking about a math test one day before the test.” A composite score was created by taking an average of all the items, with higher scores indicating higher math anxiety. Cronbach’s α for this scale was .92.

Negative Math Homework Behaviors

Parents reported child math homework behaviors using the 15 items of the Homework Problem Checklist (Anesko et al., 1987). Parents rated on a 4-point scale (1 = never to 4 = very often) how frequently their child engaged in certain behaviors while doing their math homework, such as procrastination, rushing through homework, and failure to complete homework. Example items are “Hurries through his/her math homework and makes careless mistakes”, and “Procrastinates or puts off doing his/her math homework”. A composite score was created by taking an average of all the items, with higher scores indicating more negative math homework behaviors. Cronbach’s α for this scale was .86.

Math Exam Preparation Perseverance

Parents reported their child’s perseverance in math exam preparations on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree) using 3 items from the PISA 2012 assessment (OECD, 2013): “My child is always prepared for his/her math exams”, “My child always studies hard for math quizzes”, and “My child always keeps studying until s/he understands math material.” A composite score was created by taking an average of all the items, with higher scores indicating more perseverance. Cronbach’s α for this scale was .80.

Math Extracurricular Activity Participation

Parents reported the frequency of their child’s participation in a variety of math extracurricular activities with 4 items from the PISA 2012 assessment (OECD, 2013): “Does math as an extracurricular activity”, “Takes part in math competitions”, “Attends out-of-school math lessons”, and “Participates in a mathematics club” on a 4-point scale (1 = never or rarely to 4 = always or almost always). A composite score was created by taking an average of all four items, with higher scores indicating more participation in math extracurricular activities. Cronbach’s α for this scale was .80.

Standardized Math Achievement

Math achievement was assessed using the Applied Problem and Calculation subtests from the Woodcock-Johnson IV Tests of Achievement (Schrank et al., 2014). The Applied Problems subtest evaluates students’ ability to complete applied math word problems. Students need to listen to the problem, identify the math procedure to be used, and perform the corresponding calculations. Students need to use math skills involving number concepts, arithmetic, geometry, and trigonometry to solve these applied problems. The Calculation subtest evaluates students’ ability to solve arithmetic problems (e.g., addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division), as well as geometric, trigonometric, logarithmic, and calculus operations. Each test presents items with increasing complexity. Students of different grades and achievement levels may see different items, depending on the basal and ceiling established for each individual student. All items prior to the basal were scored 1 and all items after the ceiling were scored 0. The Mathematics Cluster (comprised of the Applied Problem and Calculation subtests) W score was used for the analyses, with higher scores indicating higher math achievement. Internal consistency reliability was .86 for the Applied Problems subtest, and .89 for the Calculation subtest.

Parent-Reported Math Achievement

Parents reported their child’s math school performance using a 5-point scale (1 = F or fail, 2 = D or below average, 3 = C or average, 4 = B or good, and 5 = A or excellent).

Covariates

Child Gender

Child gender was coded as 1 = male and 2 = female.

Child Grade

Child reported their grade level ranging from third to sixth grades.

General Anxiety

Students’ general anxiety is associated with not only students’ academic performance, but also students’ math anxiety (Pellizzoni et al., 2022). As such, general anxiety was incorporated into the analyses as a covariate to allow for the estimation of the unique effect of math anxiety on math achievement beyond the effect of general anxiety. Students’ general anxiety was assessed with the 6 items of the Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (Spence, 1997). Students reported the frequency in which they experienced various general anxiety symptoms (e.g., “I worry about things”) on a 4-point scale (0 = never to 3 = always). A composite score was created by averaging the item scores, with higher scores indicating more general anxiety. Cronbach’s α was .82.

Household Income

As household income may affect the availability of each extracurricular activity to a student, household income was incorporated into the analyses as a covariate. Household income was coded as 1 = less than $20,000, 2 = $20,000 or more but less than $40,000, 3 = $40,000 or more but less than $60,000, 4 = $60,000 or more but less than $80,000, 5 = $80,000 or more but less than $100,000, 6 = $100,000 or more but less than $150,000, 7 = $150,000 or more but less than $200,000, and 8 = $200,000 or more.

Data Analytic Strategies

SPSS version 24 (IBM Corp, 2016) was used for descriptive statistics and correlations. Using Mplus 8.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2017), we tested the conceptual model (Figure 1), which was a path analysis that examined the direct and indirect effects of math anxiety on math achievement via math learning behaviors in the afterschool setting (i.e., math homework behaviors, math exam preparation, and math extracurricular activity participation). Child gender, grade level, general anxiety, and household income were included as covariates in the model. The maximum likelihood estimator was used to estimate parameters for the path model. In addition, 95% bias corrected bootstrap confidence intervals based on 5000 bootstrap samples were reported for all path estimates. Finally, missing values were dealt with using full information maximum likelihood.

Click to enlarge
jnc.11325-f1
Figure 1

Conceptual Model

Note. (S) = Standardized achievement test scores; (P) = Parent report.

Transparency and Openness

Survey measures of all the main study variables are provided in the Supplementary Material Table S1. De-identified data and analysis scripts are available at the Open Science Framework.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. On average, students reported modest levels of math anxiety. Parents reported modest levels of child negative math homework behaviors, moderate levels of perseverance in math exam preparation, low levels of extracurricular activity participation, and high levels of math achievement. All variables were distributed widely across their respective scales, suggesting a good range of variability.

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics

Variable N % missing M SD Min Max Possible
Range
Skewness Kurtosis
(C) Math anxiety 205 1% 1.92 0.54 1 4 1 – 4 0.71 0.22
(P) Negative math homework behaviors 204 1% 1.92 0.51 1 4 1 – 4 0.60 0.34
(P) Math exam preparation perseverance 205 1% 3.42 0.89 1 5 1 – 5 -0.14 -0.20
(P) Math extracurricular activities 205 1% 1.31 0.51 1 4 1 – 4 2.54 7.08
(S) Math achievement 206 1% 501.69 17.11 452 553 0.09 0.46
(P) Math achievement 205 1% 4.43 0.79 1 5 1 – 5 -1.42 1.84
(C) General anxiety 202 2% 2.10 0.69 1 4 1 – 4 0.95 0.70

Note. (C) = Child report; (P) = Parent report; (S) = Standardized achievement test.

Correlations

Table 2 presents correlations among the study variables. Math anxiety was negatively correlated with perseverance in math exam preparation, frequency of math extracurricular activity participation, and math achievement.

Table 2

Bivariate Correlations

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Child gender (1 = male, 2 = female)
2. Child grade .06
3. Household income -.03 -.04
4. (C) General anxiety .13 .05 -.01
5. (C) Math anxiety .10 .04 -.11 .42*
6. (P) Negative math homework behaviors .04 -.02 -.25* .23* .27*
7. (P) Math exam preparation perseverance -.02 .07 .16* -.15* -.16* -.61*
8. (P) Math extracurricular activities -.04 -.14* .16* -.15* -.21* -.24* .21*
9. (S) Math achievement -.09 .37* .31* -.21* -.28* -.39* .24* .30*
10. (P) Math achievement -.17* -.00 .35* -.22* -.33* -.54* .43* .25* .53*

Note. (C) = Child report; (P) = Parent report; (S) = Standardized achievement test.

*indicates statistical significance under type I error rate of .05.

Math anxiety was positively correlated with negative math homework behaviors. More perseverance in math exam preparation and more frequent participation in math extracurricular activities were correlated with higher math achievement, whereas more negative math homework behaviors were correlated with lower math achievement. Students’ learning behaviors in the various afterschool settings were also modestly to moderately correlated with one another.

Path Analysis

The path model examining the mediating roles of math learning behaviors in the association between math anxiety and math achievement is shown in Figure 1. Standardized path coefficients and 95% bias corrected bootstrap confidence intervals for the direct predictive paths are presented in Figure 2.

Click to enlarge
jnc.11325-f2
Figure 2

Standardized Path Coefficients and 95% Bias Corrected Bootstrap Confidence Intervals for Direct Predictive Paths

Note. (S) = Standardized achievement test scores; (P) = Parent report. Correlational paths and effects of covariates are not shown in the figure for simplicity of presentation. Significant (i.e., 95% bias corrected bootstrap confidence interval does not include zero) path estimates are bolded.

Standardized path coefficients, standard errors, and 95% bias corrected bootstrap confidence intervals for all paths are presented in Table 3. This path model is saturated. After controlling for child gender, grade level, general anxiety, and household income, there were negative total effects of math anxiety on both the standardized math achievement test scores, β = -0.19, 95% CI [-0.32, -0.05], and parent-reported math achievement, β = -0.26, 95% CI [-0.42, -0.09]. This suggests that children with higher math anxiety exhibited lower math achievement. In addition, math anxiety positively predicted negative math homework behaviors, β = 0.18, 95% CI [0.04, 0.32], and negatively predicted participation in math extracurricular activities, β = -0.17, 95% CI [-0.30, -0.03]. That is, children with higher math anxiety showed more negative math homework behaviors and participated less frequently in math extracurricular activities. Math anxiety did not predict levels of perseverance in math exam preparation, β = -0.11, 95% CI [-0.27, 0.05]. Math anxiety negatively indirectly predicted both standardized math achievement test scores, β = -0.05, 95% CI [-0.11, -0.01], and parent-reported math achievement, β = -0.06, 95% CI [-0.13, -0.01], via negative math homework behaviors. Math anxiety also negatively indirectly predicted standardized math achievement test scores via math extracurricular activity participation, β = -0.04, 95% CI [-0.11, -0.01]. These findings indicated that negative math homework behaviors and participation in math extracurricular activities partially contributed to the negative association between math anxiety and math achievement. Math exam preparation did not uniquely mediate the association between math anxiety and math achievement, β = 0.01, 95% CI [-0.00, 0.04] for standardized math achievement test scores; β = -0.02, 95% CI [-0.05, 0.01] for parent-reported math achievement.1

Table 3

Standardized Path Coefficients and 95% Bias Corrected Bootstrap Confidence Intervals

Path Coefficient Standard error 95% Bias Corrected Bootstrap CI
Predictors → mediators
MA → Negative math homework behaviors 0.18 .07 [0.04, 0.32]
MA → Math exam preparation perseverance -0.11 .08 [-0.27, 0.05]
MA → Math extracurricular activities -0.17 .07 [-0.30, -0.03]
Mediators → outcomes
Negative math homework behaviors → (S) Math achievement -0.25 .08 [-0.41, -0.10]
Math exam preparation perseverance → (S) Math achievement -0.05 .07 [-0.19, 0.07]
Math extracurricular activities → (S) Math achievement 0.26 .08 [0.11, 0.40]
Negative math homework behaviors → (P) Math achievement -0.33 .09 [-0.49, -0.16]
Math exam preparation perseverance → (P) Math achievement 0.15 .06 [0.02, 0.26]
Math extracurricular activities → (P) Math achievement 0.06 .04 [-0.02, 0.14]
Direct effects of predictor on outcomes
MA → (S) Math achievement -0.11 .07 [-0.23, 0.02]
MA → (P) Math achievement -0.17 .08 [-0.33, -0.03]
Indirect effects of predictor on outcomes
MA → Negative math homework behaviors → (S) Math achievement -0.05 .02 [-0.11, -0.01]
MA → Math exam preparation perseverance → (S) Math achievement 0.01 .01 [-0.01, 0.04]
MA → Math extracurricular activities → (S) Math achievement -0.04 .03 [-0.11, -0.01]
MA → Negative math homework behaviors → (P) Math achievement -0.06 .03 [-0.13, -0.01]
MA → Math exam preparation perseverance → (P) Math achievement -0.02 .02 [-0.05, 0.01]
MA → Math extracurricular activities → (P) Math achievement -0.01 .01 [-0.04, 0.00]
Total effects of predictor on outcomes
MA → (S) Math achievement -0.19 .07 [-0.32, -0.05]
MA → (P) Math achievement -0.26 .08 [-0.42, -0.09]
Effects of covariates
General anxiety → MA 0.41 .07 [0.28, 0.54]
Child gender → MA 0.05 .07 [-0.08, 0.18]
Child grade → MA 0.03 .06 [-0.09, 0.15]
Household income → MA -0.09 .06 [-0.20, 0.03]
General anxiety → Negative math homework behaviors 0.15 .07 [0.01, 0.29]
Child gender → Negative math homework behaviors -0.01 .07 [-0.15, 0.12]
Child grade → Negative math homework behaviors -0.04 .07 [-0.17, 0.09]
Household income → Negative math homework behaviors -0.23 .07 [-0.36, -0.10]
General anxiety → Math exam preparation perseverance -0.11 .08 [-0.27, 0.04]
Child gender → Math exam preparation perseverance -0.01 .07 [-0.15, 0.13]
Child grade → Math exam preparation perseverance 0.09 .07 [-0.05, 0.23]
Household income → Math exam preparation perseverance 0.15 .08 [-0.01, 0.29]
General anxiety → Math extracurricular activities -0.06 .07 [-0.18, 0.08]
Child gender → Math extracurricular activities -0.01 .07 [-0.14, 0.13]
Child grade → Math extracurricular activities -0.12 .06 [-0.23, 0.01]
Household income → Math extracurricular activities 0.14 .07 [-0.01, 0.25]
General anxiety → (S) Math achievement -0.10 .06 [-0.22, 0.02]
Child gender → (S) Math achievement -0.06 .06 [-0.16, 0.05]
Child grade → (S) Math achievement 0.44 .06 [0.33, 0.54]
Household income → (S) Math achievement 0.22 .06 [0.10, 0.33]
General anxiety → (P) Math achievement -0.03 .07 [-0.17, 0.11]
Child gender → (P) Math achievement -0.11 .06 [-0.21, 0.01]
Child grade → (P) Math achievement 0.03 .07 [-0.10, 0.16]
Household income → (P) Math achievement 0.21 .05 [0.10, 0.32]
Correlations between mediators
Negative math homework behaviors & Math exam preparation perseverance -0.60 .05 [-0.68, -0.50]
Math exam preparation perseverance & Math extracurricular activities 0.15 .07 [0.01, 0.28]
Negative math homework behaviors & Math extracurricular activities -0.17 .06 [-0.29, -0.04]
Correlations between outcomes
(S) Math achievement & (P) Math achievement 0.38 .07 [0.22, 0.51]

Note. (P) = Parent report; (S) = Standardized achievement test; MA = math anxiety. Statistical significance is defined by a 95% bias corrected bootstrap confidence interval that does not include zero. Significant path estimates are in bold face.

Discussion

Building on the learning avoidance model, the present study sought to examine the direct and indirect effects of math anxiety on math achievement by way of learning avoidance in diverse afterschool learning contexts. Overall, our findings suggest that students with higher math anxiety had lower math achievement, and this negative association was partially mediated by negative math homework behaviors and math extracurricular activity participation.

Overall, we found that math anxiety and math achievement were modestly negatively correlated, a finding that is highly consistent with the existing literature (Barroso et al., 2021; Namkung et al., 2019). In terms of learning behaviors in various contexts, higher math anxiety was associated with more negative math homework behaviors and less frequent participation in math extracurricular activities. These findings are consistent with our hypothesis, which are also in line with the literature suggesting lower math homework effort and less frequent participation in math extracurricular activities from students who are less interested in math and who value math less (LeFevre et al., 1992; Trautwein et al., 2009). These findings are, however, in contrast to a recent study which did not find an association between math anxiety and the amount of time students spent on homework (Hasty et al., 2021). Together, these findings suggest that highly math anxious students may spend as much time as their non-anxious peers doing homework, but they may not be as engaged or efficient during their homework time as their non-anxious peers.

Additionally, we found that math anxiety was not associated with the level of perseverance in math exam preparation. This finding is at odds with a recent study which found that highly math anxious high school students were less likely to use effortful study strategies during exam preparation (Jenifer et al., 2022). This inconsistency may be because these two studies focused on different aspects of exam preparation, with the present study focusing on the overall perseverance and the Jenifer et al. (2022) study focusing on strategy choices. It is also possible that exams are less frequent and often of lower stake in earlier educational stages as compared to high school. As such, there may be a lower pressure and expectation for younger students to prepare for exams at home. This may have contributed to explaining why exam preparation accounted for the association between math anxiety and math performance among high school students in the Jenifer et al. (2022) study, but not in the current sample of 3rd to 6th graders.

Furthermore, our findings showed that the negative association between math anxiety and math achievement was partially mediated by homework behaviors and participation in math extracurricular activities. Specifically, students with higher math anxiety participated less frequently in math extracurricular activities and exhibited more avoidant homework behaviors such as procrastination, attempts to rush through homework, and failures to complete homework than their peers with lower math anxiety. These differences in homework behaviors and extracurricular activity participation were, in turn, associated with achievement differences among students with different math anxiety levels. These findings are consistent with a few studies that support the learning avoidance model (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001; Hembree, 1990; Jenifer et al., 2022; LeFevre et al., 1992; Quintero et al., 2022). Our finding extends the existing literature in several important ways. First, math anxiety-related learning avoidance is not only observed in high school and college students (e.g., Hembree, 1990; Jenifer et al., 2022; LeFevre et al., 1992); it is also observable in early educational stages among elementary and middle school students. Second, highly math anxious students exhibit avoidance in not only formal math learning contexts (e.g., math class; Quintero et al., 2022), but also informal math learning contexts including the afterschool setting. Third, it is not the amount of time students devote to afterschool learning that contributes to their undesired performance (Hasty et al., 2021); rather, it is the quality of time spent on homework that critically differentiates students with different levels of math anxiety in their math performance.

Finally, our findings point to the contextual specificity of learning avoidance. In contrast to homework behaviors and extracurricular activity participation, lower levels of perseverance in exam preparation did not mediate the association between math anxiety and math achievement. Therefore, it is the accumulation of effort in everyday learning activities such as homework and participation in organized extracurricular activities, not the occasional effort spurt in activities such as exam preparation, that differentiates students with high vs. low math anxiety in their achievement.

It is worth noting that participation in math extracurricular activities only mediated the association between math anxiety and standardized math achievement test scores, but not the association between math anxiety and parent-reported math grades. This is consistent with the literature on the differences between school grades and standardized tests as achievement measures. Specifically, although students’ course grades largely capture what is learned in the classroom, standardized achievement tests additionally assess students’ academic skills beyond their curricula (Popham, 1999; Willingham et al., 2002). This may explain why participation in extracurricular activities was associated with standardized achievement test scores more strongly than it was associated with course grades (Duckworth et al., 2012).

Limitations

The current findings should be interpreted in light of several limitations. First, the present study was a cross-sectional study. Therefore, the associations in the path model should not be interpreted as causal effects. For example, despite the predictive path from participation in extracurricular activities to math achievement, participation in certain extracurricular activities (e.g., taking part in math competition and taking out-of-school math lessons) may depend on students’ levels of math achievement. Future longitudinal studies are needed to elucidate the temporal precedence among these variables. Second, although math achievement was measured using both course grades and standardized test scores, parent-reported course grades may be subject to reporter-bias such as the social desirability effect. If possible, future studies would benefit from using school records to assess students’ classroom achievement. Thirdly, only parent reports were used to measure students’ afterschool learning behaviors. It is possible that students’ subjective experience of their effort and perseverance may not be completely captured by parental observation. Relatedly, although learning avoidance behaviors are conceptualized as students’ voluntary avoidance, the current operationalization of some avoidance behaviors may also capture involuntary avoidance. For example, parents and other adults may decide what extracurricular activities their children could take part in especially in early educational stages. Parental report of their child’s homework behaviors may also relate to parents’ roles in homework as well as their own perceptions of family schedules around homework time. Therefore, alternative methods, such as student self-report, in-person interviews, and observations should be employed in future studies to better understand students’ perspectives on their own learning behaviors for young learners. Finally, the present sample only included a small proportion of 3rd graders. Further investigations are needed to evaluate the generalizability of the present findings to early elementary school students.

Conclusions and Implications

In summary, the present study provided evidence supporting the learning avoidance model in the association between math anxiety and math achievement. Our findings demonstrate that negative homework behaviors (e.g., complaints, procrastination, rushing through homework, and failure to complete homework) and less frequent participation in math extracurricular activities may contribute to explaining the undesired math achievement outcomes among math anxious students in early educational stages. Homework provides students with regular opportunities to review the learned materials and practice problem solving in their own pace, which are instrumental for knowledge consolidation and integration (Cooper, 2007). Academic extracurricular activities also provide students with additional opportunities to develop their cognitive and noncognitive skills conducive to academic learning (Covay & Carbonaro, 2010; Knifsend & Graham, 2012; Morris, 2016). Unfortunately, our findings suggest that highly math anxious students may not be able to make effective use of their homework time or to actively participate in math extracurricular activities to promote their math performance. Therefore, effective practices that aim to promote math achievement among highly math anxious students need to address learning behaviors both in and after class. These may include practices that foster positive homework behaviors such as promoting the intrinsic and utility value of homework, training students how to self-regulate, and designing afterschool environments that are conducive to independent learning. Such efforts may prove fruitful in mitigating the negative link between math anxiety and math achievement.

Notes

1) We conducted the analyses using a subsample that excluded 3rd graders because there were only four 3rd graders in the sample. We also conducted the analyses using a subsample excluding students with developmental disabilities. Results from both sets of analyses remain essentially the same as the results obtained using the full sample. These results are reported in the Supplementary Material Tables S2 and S3.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Science Foundation grant CAREER DRL-1750025 and CAREER DRL-2244865 awarded to Zhe Wang. The content is the responsibility of the authors and does not represent the official views of the funding agency.

Acknowledgments

We thank the participating families, research assistants, and funding agency.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Author Contributions

Z.W. designed the study. S.S. and Z.W. conducted the analyses and drafted the manuscript. T.L. and M.Q. provided constructive feedback throughout the development of the manuscript.

Ethics Statement

This study has been carried out in accordance with relevant ethical principles and standards. Ethical Approval was obtained via Institutional Review Board at Texas Tech University and Texas A&M University.

Data Availability

The de-identified data and analysis code for this study are publicly available (see Song et al., 2023a).

Supplementary Materials

The Supplementary Materials include the following items:

  • De-identified data and analysis code (Song et al., 2023a)

  • Measures of the main study constructs, as well as results obtained from the sample excluding 3rd graders and students with developmental disabilities (Song et al., 2023b).

Index of Supplementary Materials

  • Song, S., Li, T., Quintero, M., & Wang, Z. (2023a). Supplementary materials to "The link between math anxiety and math achievement: The role of afterschool learning" [Research data and analysis code]. OSF. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/CRJM4

  • Song, S., Li, T., Quintero, M., & Wang, Z. (2023b). Supplementary materials to "The link between math anxiety and math achievement: The role of afterschool learning" [Additional measures and results]. PsychOpen GOLD. https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.13563

References

  • Ainley, M. D. (1993). Styles of engagement with learning: Multidimensional assessment of their relationship with strategy use and school achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 395-405. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.395

  • Anesko, K. M., Schoiock, G., Ramirez, R., & Levine, F. M. (1987). The homework problem checklist: Assessing children’s homework difficulties. Behavioral Assessment, 9(2), 179-185.

  • Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(2), 224-237. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.224

  • Barroso, C., Ganley, C. M., McGraw, A. L., Geer, E. A., Hart, S. A., & Daucourt, M. C. (2021). A meta-analysis of the relation between math anxiety and math achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 147(2), 134-168. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000307

  • Broh, B. A. (2002). Linking extracurricular programming to academic achievement: Who benefits and why? Sociology of Education, 75(1), 69-95. https://doi.org/10.2307/3090254

  • Carey, E., Hill, F., Devine, A., & Szűcs, D. (2016). The chicken or the egg? The direction of the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1987. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987

  • Cargnelutti, E., Tomasetto, C., & Passolunghi, M. C. (2017). How is anxiety related to math performance in young students? A longitudinal study of Grade 2 to Grade 3 children. Cognition and Emotion, 31(4), 755-764. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1147421

  • Chiu, L.-h., & Henry, L. L. (1990). Development and validation of the Mathematics Anxiety Scale for Children. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 23(3), 121-127.

  • Cooper, H. (2007). The battle over homework: Common ground for administrators, teachers, and parents (3rd ed.). Corwin Press. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781483329420

  • Covay, E., & Carbonaro, W. (2010). After the bell: Participation in extracurricular activities, classroom behavior, and academic achievement. Sociology of Education, 83(1), 20-45. https://doi.org/10.1177/0038040709356565

  • Duckworth, A. L., Quinn, P. D., & Tsukayama, E. (2012). What No Child Left Behind leaves behind: The roles of IQ and self-control in predicting standardized achievement test scores and report card grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(2), 439-451. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026280

  • Eckerlein, N., Roth, A., Engelschalk, T., Steuer, G., Schmitz, B., & Dresel, M. (2019). The role of motivational regulation in exam preparation: Results from a standardized diary study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 81. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00081

  • Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A., & Gable, S. (1999). Achievement goals, study strategies, and exam performance: A mediational analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 549-563. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.549

  • Fan, H., Xu, J., Cai, Z., He, J., & Fan, X. (2017). Homework and students’ achievement in math and science: A 30-year meta-analysis, 1986–2015. Educational Research Review, 20, 35-54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2016.11.003

  • Feldman, A. F., & Matjasko, J. L. (2005). The role of school-based extracurricular activities in adolescent development: A comprehensive review and future directions. Review of Educational Research, 75(2), 159-210. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543075002159

  • Fredricks, J. A. (2012). Extracurricular participation and academic outcomes: Testing the over-scheduling hypothesis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(3), 295-306. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9704-0

  • Ganley, C. M., & McGraw, A. L. (2016). The development and validation of a revised version of the Math Anxiety Scale for Young Children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 1181. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01181

  • Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Scofield, J. E. (2021). In-class attention, spatial ability, and mathematics anxiety predict across-grade gains in adolescents’ mathematics achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(4), 754-769. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000487

  • Haghighat, M. D., & Knifsend, C. A. (2019). The longitudinal influence of 10th grade extracurricular activity involvement: Implications for 12th grade academic practices and future educational attainment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48(3), 609-619. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-018-0947-x

  • Hasty, L. M., Malanchini, M., Shakeshaft, N., Schofield, K., Malanchini, M., & Wang, Z. (2021). When anxiety becomes my propeller: Mental toughness moderates the relation between academic anxiety and academic avoidance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 368-390. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12366

  • Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21(1), 33-46. https://doi.org/10.2307/749455

  • Hopko, D. R., Mahadevan, R., Bare, R. L., & Hunt, M. K. (2003). The Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale (AMAS): Construction, validity, and reliability. Assessment, 10(2), 178-182. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191103010002008

  • IBM Corp. (2016). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 24.0) [Computer software]. https://www.ibm.com/analytics/spss-statistics-software

  • Jacobs, J. E., Vernon, M. K., & Eccles, J. S. (2005). Activity choices in middle childhood: The roles of gender, self-beliefs, and parents’ influence. In J. L. Mahoney, R. W. Larson, & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Organized activities as contexts of development: Extracurricular activities, after-school and community programs (pp. 235–254). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410612748

  • Jenifer, J. B., Rozek, C. S., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2022). Effort(less) exam preparation: Math anxiety predicts the avoidance of effortful study strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151(10), 2534-2541. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001202

  • Knifsend, C. A., & Graham, S. (2012). Too much of a good thing? How breadth of extracurricular participation relates to school-related affect and academic outcomes during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(3), 379-389. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9737-4

  • LeFevre, J. A., Kulak, A. G., & Heymans, S. L. (1992). Factors influencing the selection of university majors varying in mathematical content. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 24(3), 276-289. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0078742

  • Lu, Y., Li, Q., Patrick, H., & Mantzicopoulos, P. (2021). “Math gives me a tummy ache!” Mathematics anxiety in kindergarten. Journal of Experimental Education, 89(2), 362-378. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2019.1680518

  • Lukowski, S. L., DiTrapani, J., Jeon, M., Wang, Z., Schenker, V. J., Doran, M. M., Hart, S. A., Mazzocco, M. M. M., Willcutt, E. G., Thompson, L. A., & Petrill, S. A. (2019). Multidimensionality in the measurement of math-specific anxiety and its relationship with mathematical performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 70, 228-235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.07.007

  • Luo, W., Hogan, D., Tan, L. S., Kaur, B., Ng, P. T., & Chan, M. (2014). Self‐construal and students’ math self‐concept, anxiety and achievement: An examination of achievement goals as mediators. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 17(3), 184-195. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12058

  • Luthar, S. S., Shoum, K. A., & Brown, P. J. (2006). Extracurricular involvement among affluent youth: A scapegoat for “ubiquitous achievement pressures”? Developmental Psychology, 42(3), 583-597. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.3.583

  • Ma, X., & Xu, J. (2004). The causal ordering of mathematics anxiety and mathematics achievement: A longitudinal panel analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 27(2), 165-179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.11.003

  • Mahoney, J. L., & Cairns, R. B. (1997). Do extracurricular activities protect against early school dropout? Developmental Psychology, 33(2), 241-253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.2.241

  • McGregor, H. A., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Achievement goals as predictors of achievement-relevant processes prior to task engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(2), 381-395. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.381

  • Metsäpelto, R.-L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2014). The benefits of extracurricular activities for socioemotional behavior and school achievement in middle childhood: An overview of the research. Journal for Educational Research Online, 6(3), 10-33. https://doi.org/10.25656/01:9685

  • Moneta, G. B., & Spada, M. M. (2009). Coping as a mediator of the relationships between trait intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and approaches to studying during academic exam preparation. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(5-6), 664-669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.012

  • Morris, D. S. (2016). Extracurricular activity participation in high school: Mechanisms linking participation to math achievement and 4-year college attendance. American Educational Research Journal, 53(5), 1376-1410. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831216667579

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus (Version 8.4) [Computer software]. https://www.statmodel.com

  • Namkung, J. M., Peng, P., & Lin, X. (2019). The relation between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance among school-aged students: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 89(3), 459-496. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654319843494

  • OECD. (2013). PISA 2012 Results: Ready to learn: Students’ engagement, drive and self-beliefs (Volume III). OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264201170-en

  • Pellizzoni, S., Cargnelutti, E., Cuder, A., & Passolunghi, M. C. (2022). The interplay between math anxiety and working memory on math performance: A longitudinal study. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1510(1), 132-144. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14722

  • Popham, W. J. (1999). Why standardized test scores don’t measure educational quality. Educational Leadership, 56(6), 8-15.

  • Quintero, M., Hasty, L., Li, T., Song, S., & Wang, Z. (2022). A multidimensional examination of math anxiety and engagement on math achievement. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(3), 955-973. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12482

  • Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2013). Math anxiety, working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(2), 187-202. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.664593

  • Ramirez, G., Shaw, S. T., & Maloney, E. A. (2018). Math anxiety: Past research, promising interventions, and a new interpretation framework. Educational Psychologist, 53(3), 145-164. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2018.1447384

  • Rosário, P., Núñez, J. C., Vallejo, G., Nunes, T., Cunha, J., Fuentes, S., & Valle, A. (2018). Homework purposes, homework behaviors, and academic achievement: Examining the mediating role of students’ perceived homework quality. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 53, 168-180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.04.001

  • Schrank, F. A., Mather, N., & McGrew, K. S. (2014). Woodcock-Johnson IV Tests of Achievement [Measurement instrument]. Riverside.

  • Simpkins, S. D., Vest, A. E., & Becnel, J. N. (2010). Participating in sport and music activities in adolescence: The role of activity participation and motivational beliefs during elementary school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(11), 1368-1386. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9448-2

  • Spence, S. H. (1997). Structure of anxiety symptoms among children: A confirmatory factor-analytic study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(2), 280-297. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.2.280

  • Tobias, S. (1986). Anxiety and cognitive processing of instruction. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-related cognitions in anxiety and motivation (pp. 35–54). Erlbaum.

  • Trautwein, U. (2007). The homework-achievement relation reconsidered: Differentiating homework time, homework frequency, and homework effort. Learning and Instruction, 17(3), 372-388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.02.009

  • Trautwein, U., Niggli, A., Schnyder, I., & Lüdtke, O. (2009). Between-teacher differences in homework assignments and the development of students’ homework effort, homework emotions, and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(1), 176-189. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.101.1.176

  • Willingham, W. W., Pollack, J. M., & Lewis, C. (2002). Grades and test scores: Accounting for observed differences. Journal of Educational Measurement, 39(1), 1-37. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-3984.2002.tb01133.x

  • Xu, J., Du, J., Cunha, J., & Rosário, P. (2021). Student perceptions of homework quality, autonomy support, effort, and math achievement: Testing models of reciprocal effects. Teaching and Teacher Education, 108, Article 103508. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2021.103508