Journal of Numerical Cognition https://jnc.psychopen.eu/index.php/jnc <p>Welcome to the&nbsp;<em>Journal of Numerical Cognition</em>&nbsp;(JNC).&nbsp;The main objective of this Journal is to provide a scholarly forum for the focused discussion of research into numerical cognition.&nbsp;The Journal aims to be inclusive, and takes numerical cognition to encompass for example all branches of mathematics (including spatial reasoning), and welcomes research for its quality, regardless of whether its origins are in (Cognitive) Psychology, Education, Anthropology, or other disciplines.</p> <p>JNC is a free-to-publish, open access and peer-reviewed journal publishing original studies, critical commentaries, theoretical position statements and book reviews relevant to the area. The journal emphasizes scholarship, whilst also seeking to bridge theory and practice. The journal aims to represent the vibrancy and innovation of empirical work and ideas in the field, and showcase the best contemporary research. It seeks to have a worldwide audience, which the open access model facilitates.</p> <p>JNC aims to facilitate communication not only amongst psychologists but also between psychologists and cognate disciplines. It also encourages dialogue between theory development and practice with respect to understanding numerical cognition. The objective of the journal is to provide readers with access to high-quality professional information, thus stimulating progress throughout the scientific community. Indeed JNC welcomes contributions from researchers and practitioners irrespective of their geographical location.</p> <p>JNC is indexed in&nbsp;<a title="Scopus" href="https://www.scopus.com/sourceid/21100943318" target="_blank" rel="noopener">Scopus</a>, <a href="https://eric.ed.gov/?q=source%3A%22Journal+of+Numerical+Cognition%22" target="_blank" rel="noopener">ERIC</a>, <a title="Dimensions" href="https://app.dimensions.ai/discover/publication" target="_blank" rel="noopener">Dimensions</a>,&nbsp;<a title="PubPsych" href="https://pubpsych.zpid.de/pubpsych/Search.action?search=&amp;q=ISSN=%222363-8761%22" target="_blank" rel="noopener">PubPsych</a>,&nbsp;<a title="OpenAIRE" href="https://explore.openaire.eu/" target="_blank" rel="noopener">OpenAIRE</a>,&nbsp;<a title="DOAJ" href="https://doaj.org/toc/2363-8761" target="_blank" rel="noopener">DOAJ</a>,&nbsp;<a title="BASE" href="https://www.base-search.net/Search/Results?q=coll:ftjnc" target="_blank" rel="noopener">BASE</a>,&nbsp;<a title="Google Scholar" href="https://scholar.google.com/" target="_blank" rel="noopener">Google Scholar</a>, <a href="https://app.scilit.net/sources/64238" target="_blank" rel="noopener">Scilit</a>, <a href="https://essentials.ebsco.com/search/eds?query=AND%202363-8761%20IS" target="_blank" rel="noopener">EBSCO</a>, and <a title="Genios" href="https://www.wiso-net.de/dosearch" target="_blank" rel="noopener">Genios</a>. JNC is also member of the&nbsp;<a href="https://freejournals.org/" target="_blank" rel="noopener">Free Journal Network</a> (FJN).</p> <p>JNC is listed in Scopus with an<strong> impact factor</strong> of 1.775.</p> <p><a href="https://www.the-mcls.org/" target="_blank" rel="noopener"><img class="mr-3 mb-3" style="float: left;" src="/public/journals/25/MCLS_logo.png" alt="Cover" height="104"></a> JNC is the official journal of the <a href="https://www.the-mcls.org/" target="_blank" rel="noopener">Mathematical Cognition and Learning Society (MCLS)</a>.</p> PsychOpen en-US Journal of Numerical Cognition 2363-8761 <p>Authors who publish with the <em>Journal of Numerical Cognition</em> ("<strong>the Journal</strong>") agree to the following terms:</p> <p><a href="https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" target="_blank" rel="noopener"><img style="border-width: 0; float: left; margin-right: 2em; margin-bottom: 1em;" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" alt="Creative Commons License"></a></p> <p>Articles are published under the <a href="https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" target="_blank" rel="noopener">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a> (CC BY 4.0).</p> <p>Under the CC BY license, authors retain ownership of the copyright for their article, but authors grant others permission to use the content of publications in <strong>the Journal</strong> in whole or in part provided that the original work is properly cited. Users (redistributors) of <strong>the Journal</strong> are required to cite the original source, including the author's names, <strong>the Journal</strong> as the initial source of publication, year of publication, volume number and DOI (if available).</p> <p>Authors may publish the manuscript in any other journal or medium but any such subsequent publication must include a notice that the manuscript was initially published by <strong>the Journal</strong>.</p> <p>Authors grant <strong>the Journal</strong> the right of first publication. Although authors remain the copyright owner, they grant the journal the irrevocable, nonexclusive rights to publish, reproduce, publicly distribute and display, and transmit their article or portions thereof in any manner.</p> Embodied Magnitude Processing: On the Relation Between the SNARC Effect and Perceived Reachability https://jnc.psychopen.eu/index.php/jnc/article/view/10885 <p>Magnitude information, for instance, regarding weight, distance, or velocity, is crucial for planning goal-directed interactions. Accordingly, magnitude information, including numerical magnitude, can affect actions: Responses to small numbers are faster with the left hand than the right and vice versa (hand-based SNARC effect). Previous experiments found an influence of effector placements on the SNARC effect but also an influence of the mere distance between effectors and numbers. This indicates a sensorimotor grounding of space-number processing. In the current study, we investigated this grounding by probing the SNARC effect close to and far from the hands. We used a magnitude comparison task with a fixed standard of 5 (smaller numbers 1, 2, 3, 4; larger numbers 6, 7, 8, 9) and a sagittal response arrangement to measure hand-based and sagittal SNARC effects for digits presented at different sagittal distances to the hands, i.e., in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. A significant sagittal SNARC effect was found, with the largest effect size in extrapersonal space. Meanwhile, the hand-based SNARC effect appeared only descriptively, with the largest effect size between the hands, i.e., in peripersonal space. Additionally, a purely spatial congruency effect surfaced, prioritizing responses with the hand closer to the number. Together, these results emphasize that responses in simple decision-making tasks can be influenced interactively by a multitude of task-relevant axes and relative spatial locations, including effector placement and stimulus placement, as well as number magnitude.</p> Nadine Koch Johannes Lohmann Martin V. Butz Hans-Christoph Nuerk Copyright (c) 2024 Nadine Koch, Johannes Lohmann, Martin V. Butz, Hans-Christoph Nuerk https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 2024-03-15 2024-03-15 10 1 24 10.5964/jnc.10885 Money Talks! The Role of Parents’ Discussion of Money for Preschoolers’ Math Knowledge https://jnc.psychopen.eu/index.php/jnc/article/view/11351 <p>Children’s participation in cultural, everyday practices and social interactions involving math and money can contribute to the development of their knowledge and skills in these domains. Further work is needed to uncover what features of these activities, such as aspects of the conversations that may occur (e.g., number and money talk), facilitate and/or are shaped by children’s understanding of money concepts and skills. The present study examined the extent to which parents engaged in conversations about numbers and money with their four-year-old children during pretend grocery play and the relations to children’s math skills. We found that talk about price labeling and exchanging currency or goods occurred most frequently and that money and number talk were not significantly related to children’s broader math skills. However, parents’ money talk was positively associated with children’s money-related math skills, and this association was driven by the co-occurrence of talk about money and numbers. Our results suggest that parent-child conversations in familiar contexts such as grocery shopping provide rich opportunities to discuss culturally relevant practices surrounding money and practice math skills in the context of monetary exchanges. Thus, it is critical to consider how existing family practices and everyday contexts support children’s early math learning.</p> Shirley Duong Leanne E. Elliott Olivia Sidoti Heather J. Bachman Melissa E. Libertus Elizabeth Votruba-Drzal Copyright (c) 2024 Shirley Duong, Leanne E. Elliott, Olivia Sidoti, Heather J. Bachman, Melissa E. Libertus, Elizabeth Votruba-Drzal https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 2024-03-15 2024-03-15 10 1 19 10.5964/jnc.11351 A Brief, Multiple-Choice Assessment of Mature Number Sense Is Strongly Correlated With More Resource-Intensive Measures https://jnc.psychopen.eu/index.php/jnc/article/view/12679 <p>Students who exhibit mature number sense make sense of numbers and operations, use reasoning to notice patterns, and flexibly choose effective problem-solving strategies (McIntosh et al., 1997, https://ro.ecu.edu.au/ecuworks/6819). Due to its dispositional nature, mature number sense is typically measured through in-depth interviews or tests of strategy usage. Yet, the lack of an efficient, rigorously developed measure has made it difficult to collect systematic, replicable evidence on students’ mature number sense. To address this, we developed a brief assessment of mature number sense. The present study provides additional convergent evidence of validity for this measure with US students in grades 3-8 (8–14 years old). We compared middle school (N = 40) and upper elementary school (N = 41) scores from the brief assessment with an established, time-intensive measure (Yang, 2019, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-018-9874-8) and an in-depth interview of student strategy usage (Markovits &amp; Sowder, 1994, https://doi.org/10.2307/749290). We found strong correlations (r &gt; 0.7) across all three measures, and this held even when controlling for students’ arithmetic scores (pr &gt; 0.6). Researchers and educators can now use the brief assessment to investigate students’ mathematical thinking and advance knowledge of a key aspect of mathematical cognition.</p> Patrick K. Kirkland Claire Guang Chineme Otuonye Nicole M. McNeil Copyright (c) 2024 Patrick K. Kirkland, Claire Guang, Chineme Otuonye, Nicole M. McNeil https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 2024-03-15 2024-03-15 10 1 18 10.5964/jnc.12679 The Ecological Validity of Picture SFON Tasks https://jnc.psychopen.eu/index.php/jnc/article/view/11055 <p>Research has identified that children differ in the extent to which they spontaneously focus on numerical aspects of the environment (Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity, SFON) and that this correlates with their mathematics achievement. It is assumed that the mechanism underpinning this relationship is that children who spontaneously focus on numerical features of their environment will experience more self-initiated practice with number concepts. We explored this mechanism by investigating whether 4- to 5-year-old children’s verbal SFON scores on a picture description task related to their spontaneous focusing on number while engaged in play activities with their parent. We found that the scores derived from a picture description task were strongly correlated with the scores derived from the play sessions, rₛ = .638, 95% CI [.433, .781], providing evidence for this mechanism. We further investigated the role that verbal abilities may play in children’s performance on the picture description task, finding that general verbal abilities were not associated with verbal SFON scores. These results contribute to our understanding of the role played by verbal SFON tendencies in explaining differences in numerical development, and demonstrate the ecological validity of SFON picture tasks.</p> Sophie Batchelor Camilla Gilmore Jayne Spiller Matthew Inglis Copyright (c) 2024 Sophie Batchelor, Camilla Gilmore, Jayne Spiller, Matthew Inglis https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 2024-03-15 2024-03-15 10 1 13 10.5964/jnc.11055 Assessment of Computation Competence and Non-Count Strategy Use in Addition and Subtraction in Grade 1 https://jnc.psychopen.eu/index.php/jnc/article/view/12633 <p>Computation competence (CC) in simple addition and subtraction using non-counting (NC) strategies is an important learning objective in Grade 1 mathematics but many children, especially low achievers in mathematics, struggle to acquire these skills. To provide these students with the support they need, it is important to have valid and reliable tools for assessing progress in CC and NC strategy use. Developing an assessment instrument for use in Grade 1, when some children start the year unable to solve any problems, is challenging, as is ensuring measurement invariance over a school year when children generally make large achievement gains. This paper presents a new assessment tool for CC and NC strategy use in Grade 1 that was tested in a longitudinal study with N = 1,017 children. Analyses using the Rasch model revealed acceptable mean square scores (MNSQ 0.83 – 1.20). Warm’s Weighted Likelihood Estimate (WLE) reliability scores were acceptable (pre-test .77; post-test .87). Measurement invariance over time was given. The instrument is promising for assessing CC and NC strategy use efficiently and accurately in Grade 1.</p> Delia Leuenberger Elisabeth Moser Opitz Noemi Gloor Copyright (c) 2024 Delia Leuenberger, Elisabeth Moser Opitz, Noemi Gloor https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 2024-04-17 2024-04-17 10 1 16 10.5964/jnc.12633 Understanding the Role of Working Memory and Phonological Memory in Mathematics and Response to Intervention for Emergent Bilingual Kindergartners https://jnc.psychopen.eu/index.php/jnc/article/view/11635 <p>This study explores how kindergarten students from a multilingual sample (n = 131) representing 23 different languages differ in response to intervention, based on their skill in mathematics and domain general cognitive skills. Analyses for this study indicate significant correlations between initial math skill, phonological memory, working memory, and language proficiency. There was no statistically significant relationship demonstrated between gains in mathematics and phonological memory, working memory, and language proficiency. No moderation effect was found between domain general skills and response to math intervention. Implications of this work will inform development and delivery of math interventions for multilingual students in kindergarten.</p> Madison A. Cook Keith Smolkowski Lina Shanley Joanna Hermida Sylvia Linan-Thompson Christian T. Doabler Ben Clarke Copyright (c) 2024 Madison A. Cook, Keith Smolkowski, Lina Shanley, Joanna Hermida, Sylvia Linan-Thompson, Christian T. Doabler, Ben Clarke https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 2024-04-17 2024-04-17 10 1 25 10.5964/jnc.11635